We’re all familiar with the experience of time flying…and dragging.
Now scientists have found that our sense of how quickly time is passing is partly caused by changes in our bodies.
They found that greater activity of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) – i.e. increased heart rate and increased electricity conductance – had a ‘small, but significant’ effect on the feeling of time passing more quickly.
It’s thought to be the first time that these objective measures of physiological arousal have been studied in relation to time perception.
Scientists have found that our sense of how quickly time is passing is partly caused by changes in our bodies (stock image)
Researchers asked 67 people to complete a normal day’s activities while wearing sensors that measured how their sympathetic nervous system was behaving.
They wore a heart rate monitor as well as a skin sensor that measured minute changes in the skin’s conductance of electricity caused by the activity of the sweat glands.
The participants were asked to report, hourly, how fast or slowly they felt the last 60 minutes had gone by compared to normal.
The researchers then analysed the participants’ biological data together with their subjective reports.
‘Studies have generally demonstrated that boredom, social isolation, and stress are associated with a slowing of the passage of time, whereas greater social satisfaction and low stress are associated with time passing more quickly,’ they wrote in their research paper.
‘On the basis of these findings, it is often assumed that changes in physiological arousal, which often accompany fluctuations of self-reported emotions, play an important role in time distortion, but to date this inference has not been tested during real-world activity, using objective measures of physiological arousal.’
The researchers found that greater activity of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) – i.e. increased heart rate and increased electricity conductance – had a ‘small, but significant’ effect on the feeling of time passing more quickly (stock image)
Lead author of the study, Dr Ruth Ogden, from Liverpool John Moores University, said: ‘The results show, for the first time that increases in SNS activity can significantly speed up our subjective experience of time – being aroused really does make time pass more quickly.
‘When an individual experiences a significant increase in heart rate or skin conductance this can make time seem like it is passing up to 10 per cent faster.’
While this is a relatively small effect, it may be due to the participants having quite ‘normal’ days.
If they’d had been experiencing more highs and lows it’s likely the effect would have been greater, and there is thought to be an evolutionary benefit behind this, she explained.
‘The work I have done in the lab using electric shocks and other aversive stimuli has shown much larger effects of SNS activity on time experience,’ said Dr Ogden, whose findings are published in the journal Nature: Scientific Reports.
‘When we think about why humans would experience distortions to time, one idea is that there is some sort of evolutionary benefit to having a flexible timing system – think back to being a cave man, having “extra time” may have provided some survival advantage.
‘Assuming this evolutionary survival account is correct then it wouldn’t make sense to have a timing system that waxed and waned with every small change in arousal.
‘Instead it would need to be a system which reacted to large changes – fight or flight.
‘In our study, participants were mainly students going about their normal daily lives – there really weren’t that many life or death situations and maybe this limited the role of arousal in time experience.
‘We are testing this idea at the moment using a VR environment in which participants are exposed to significant virtual threat.’
The sense that time is passing quicker or slower than it really is can have significant effects on people’s wellbeing, the researchers said.
Feeling as though we do not have enough time is linked to ‘lower life satisfaction, poorer health outcomes, and risky decision-making’, they said, while for medical patients and the elderly, ‘distortions to time are associated with distress, symptom exacerbation and impaired social interaction’.